The effect of self-awareness on the ability to recognize personal motion
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.14198/jhse.2022.174.15Keywords:
Areas of interest, Human gait, Monitoring of performance, Movement perception, Point-light videosAbstract
The purpose of this study was to examine the abilities of athletes with specific gait training (i.e., runners) discriminating personal gait from point-light videos were compared to athletes for whom gait is not a trained aspect of the sport (i.e., swimmers), and that of a control. It was hypothesized that runners would discriminate their gait among different individuals with greater accuracy than the other two groups and that runners would also devote more attention to the lower extremities for recognition indications. Results showed that runners group recognized themselves more often than the other two groups (p = .048, η2 = 0.18), and that runners allotted more visual attention to the lower extremities (p < .05, ηp 2 = 0.16) when viewing the point-light videos than the other groups. The findings were consistent with other investigations with point-light video representations of movement and suggest that experience and training lead to movement self-awareness that is both recognizable and accessible by a performer.
Funding
Western Illinois University grantDownloads
References
Ahlstrom, V., Blake, R., & Ahlstrom U. (1997). Perception of biological motion. Perception, 26, 1539-1548. https://doi.org/10.1068/p261539
Bahill A. T., & LaRitz T. (1984). Why can't batters keep their eyes on the ball? American Scientist. 72, 249-253.
Bardi L., Di Giorgio, E., Lunghi, M., Troje, N. F., & Simion, F. (2015). Walking direction triggers visuo-spatial orientation in 6-month-old infants and adults: An eye tracking study. Cognition, 141, 112-120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.04.014
Bertenthal, B. L. & Pinto, J. (1994). Global processing of biological motions. Psychological Science, 5, 221-225. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00504.x
Blake, J. E., & Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 47-73. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190152
Brownlow, S., Dixon, A. R. Egbert, C. A., & Radcliffe, R. D. (1997). Perception of movement and dancer characteristics from point-light displays of dance. The Psychological Record, 47, 411-421. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395235
Clark, T. J., Bradshaw, M .F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., & Rose, D. (2005). Perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception, 34, 1171-11180. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5203
Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353-356. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03337021
Ditrich, W. H., Troscianko, T. Lea, S. E., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25(6), 727-738. https://doi.org/10.1068/p250727
Duval, T. S., & Lalwani, N. (1999). Objective self-awareness and causal attributions for self-standard discrepancies: Changing self or changing standards of correctness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1220-1229. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299258004
Duval, T. S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York, NY: Academic.
Helsen, W. F., Starkes, J. L., & Hodges, N. J. (1998). Team sports and the theory of deliberate practice. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 20(1),12-34. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.20.1.12
Horst, F., Mildner, M., & Schollhorn, W. I. (2017). One-year persistence of individual gait patterns identified in a follow-up study - a call for individualized diagnosis and therapy. Gait Posture, 58, 476-48c. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.09.003
Hug, F., Vogel, C., Tucker, K., Dorel, S., Deschamps, T., La Carpentier, E. & Lacoupaille, L. (2019). Individuals have unique muscle activation signatures as revealed during gait and pedaling. Journal of Applied Physiology, 127, 1165-1174. https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.01101.2018
Hunfalvay, M., Roberts, C. M., Ryan, W., Murray, N., Tabana, J., & Martin, C. (2017). Team sports and the theory of deliberate practice. International Journal of Sport Science, 7(6),215-222.
Ikeda, H., Watanabe, K. (2009) Anger and happiness are linked differently to the explicit detection of biological motion. Perception 38(7), 1002-1011. https://doi.org/10.1068/p6250
Jacobs, A., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Walking perception by walking observers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 157-169. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.157
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14(2), 201-211. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212378
Kato, T., & Fukuda, T. (2002). Visual search strategies of baseball players: eye movements during the preparatory phase of batting. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94(2), 380-386. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.2002.94.2.380
Khromov, N., Korotin, A., Lange, A., Stepanov, A., Burnacev, E., & Somov, A. (2018). Esports athletes and players: A comparative study. Newzoo,5, 1-11.
Knoblich, G., & Flach, R. (2001). Predicting the effects of actions: Interactions of perceptions and action. Psychological Science, 12(6), 467-472. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00387
Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Recognition of self-generated actions from kinematic displays of drawings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 456-465. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.456
Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21(6), 575-580. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198740
Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 210-220. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.210
Mann, D. L., Spratford, W., & Abernethy, B. (2013). The head tracks and gaze predicts: How the world's best batters hit a ball. PLoS ONE, 8(3). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0058289
Mather, G., Redford, K. & West, S. (1992). Low level visual processing of biological motion. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 249, 149-155. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1992.0097
Mather, G., & West, S. (1993). Recognition of animal locomotion from dynamic point-light delays. Perception, 22, 759-766. https://doi.org/10.1068/p220759
Muraskin, J., Dodhia, S., Lieberman, G., Garcia, J. O., Verstynen, J. M., Sherwin, J., & Sajda, P. (2016). Brain dynamics of post-task resting state are influenced by expertise: Insight from baseball players. Human Brain Mapping, 37(12),4454-4471. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23321
Norman, J. F., Payto, S. M., Long, J. R., & Hawkes, L. M. (2004). Aging and perception of biological motion. Psychology of Aging, 19, 219-225. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.219
Nugent, F. J., Comyns, T. M. & Warrington, G. D. (2017). Quality versus quantity debate in swimming: Perceptions and training practices of expert swimming coaches. Journal of Human Kinetics, 57, 147-158. https://doi.org/10.1515/hukin-2017-0056
Papin, J. P., Naureils, P., & Santucci, G. (1980). Pickup of visual information by the pilot during a ground control approach in a fighter aircraft simulator. Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine. 51(5), 463-469.
Pataky, T. C., Mu, T., Bosch, K., Rosenbaum, D., & Goulermas, J. Y. (2012). Gait recognition: highly unique dynamic plantar pressure patters among 104 individuals. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 9, 790-800. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2011.0430
Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). The visual perception of human and animal motion in point-light displays. Social Neuroscience, 4(4), 332-346. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910902826820
Pollick, F. E., Kay, J. W., Heim, K., & Stringer, R. (2005). Gender recognition from point-light walkers. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance, 31(6), 1247-1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1247
Prinz, W. (1997). Perceptions and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129-154. https://doi.org/10.1080/713752551
Saunders, D. R., Williamson, D. K., & Troje, N. F. (2010). Gaze patterns during perception of direction and gender from biological motion. Journal of Vision,10(11). https://doi.org/10.1167/10.11.9
Savelsbergh, G., Van der Kamp, J., Williams, A., & Ward, P. (2005). Anticipation and visual search behavior in expert soccer goalkeepers. Ergonomics, 48, 1686-1697. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140130500101346
Seya, Y., Nakayasu, H., & Patterson, P. (2008). Visual search of trained and untrained drivers in a driving simulator. Japanese Psychological Research, 50(4), 242-252. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.2008.00380.x
Shank, M. D., & Haywood, K. M. (1987). Eye movements when viewing a baseball pitch. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 64, 1191-1197. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1987.64.3c.1191
Sharma, D. A., Chevidikunnan, M. F., Khan, F. R., & Gaowgzeh, R. A. (2016). Effectiveness of knowledge of result and knowledge of performance in the learning of a skilled motor activity by healthy young adults. Journal of Physical Therapy Science, 28(5), 1482-1486. https://doi.org/10.1589/jpts.28.1482
Shiffrar, M., & Freyd, J. J. (1990). Apparent motion of the human body. Psychological Science, 1(4), 257-264. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00210.x
Silvia, P. J., & Duval, T. S. (2001). Objective self-awareness theory: Recent progress and enduring problems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 230-241. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0503_4
Slowinski, T., Zhai, C., Alderisio, F., Salesse, R., Gueugnon, M., Marin, L., Bardy, B. G. di Bernardo, M.& Tsaneva-Atansova, K. (2016). Dynamic similarity promotes interpersonal coordination in joint action. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 13, 20151093. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2015.1093
Takahashi, K., Fukuda, H., Ikeda, H., Doi, H., Watanabe, K., Ueda, K., & Shinohara, K. (2011). Roles of the upper and lower bodies in direction discrimination of point-light walkers. Journal of Vision, 11(14), 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1167/11.14.8
Viviani, P., & Stucchi, N. (1992). Biological movements look uniform: Evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 603-623. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.603
Wallace, S. A. & Hagler, R. W. (1997). Knowledge of performance and the learning of a closed motor skill. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 50, 265-271. https://doi.org/10.1080/10671315.1979.10615609
Young, B. W., & Salmela, J. H. (2010). Examination of practice activities related to the acquisition of elite performance in Canadian middle-distance running. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 41, 73-90.
Zubiaur, G. M., Ona, A. & Delgado, S. J. (1999). Learning volleyball serves: A preliminary study of the effects of knowledge of performance and of results. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 89, 223-232. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1999.89.1.223
Downloads
Statistics
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2018 Journal of Human Sport and Exercise
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
Each author warrants that his or her submission to the Work is original and that he or she has full power to enter into this agreement. Neither this Work nor a similar work has been published elsewhere in any language nor shall be submitted for publication elsewhere while under consideration by JHSE. Each author also accepts that the JHSE will not be held legally responsible for any claims of compensation.
Authors wishing to include figures or text passages that have already been published elsewhere are required to obtain permission from the copyright holder(s) and to include evidence that such permission has been granted when submitting their papers. Any material received without such evidence will be assumed to originate from the authors.
Please include at the end of the acknowledgements a declaration that the experiments comply with the current laws of the country in which they were performed. The editors reserve the right to reject manuscripts that do not comply with the abovementioned requirements. The author(s) will be held responsible for false statements or failure to fulfill the above-mentioned requirements.
This title is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International license (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0).
You are free to share, copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format. The licensor cannot revoke these freedoms as long as you follow the license terms under the following terms:
Attribution — You must give appropriate credit, provide a link to the license, and indicate if changes were made. You may do so in any reasonable manner, but not in any way that suggests the licensor endorses you or your use.
NonCommercial — You may not use the material for commercial purposes.
NoDerivatives — If you remix, transform, or build upon the material, you may not distribute the modified material.
No additional restrictions — You may not apply legal terms or technological measures that legally restrict others from doing anything the license permits.
Notices:
You do not have to comply with the license for elements of the material in the public domain or where your use is permitted by an applicable exception or limitation.
No warranties are given. The license may not give you all of the permissions necessary for your intended use. For example, other rights such as publicity, privacy, or moral rights may limit how you use the material.
Transfer of Copyright
In consideration of JHSE’s publication of the Work, the authors hereby transfer, assign, and otherwise convey all copyright ownership worldwide, in all languages, and in all forms of media now or hereafter known, including electronic media such as CD-ROM, Internet, and Intranet, to JHSE. If JHSE should decide for any reason not to publish an author’s submission to the Work, JHSE shall give prompt notice of its decision to the corresponding author, this agreement shall terminate, and neither the author nor JHSE shall be under any further liability or obligation.
Each author certifies that he or she has no commercial associations (e.g., consultancies, stock ownership, equity interest, patent/licensing arrangements, etc.) that might pose a conflict of interest in connection with the submitted article, except as disclosed on a separate attachment. All funding sources supporting the Work and all institutional or corporate affiliations of the authors are acknowledged in a footnote in the Work.
Each author certifies that his or her institution has approved the protocol for any investigation involving humans or animals and that all experimentation was conducted in conformity with ethical and humane principles of research.
Competing Interests
Biomedical journals typically require authors and reviewers to declare if they have any competing interests with regard to their research.
JHSE require authors to agree to Copyright Notice as part of the submission process.